000 | 01741nam a22002537a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | ZW-GwMSU | ||
005 | 20250305070938.0 | ||
008 | 250305b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
022 | _a00935301 | ||
040 |
_aMSU _bEnglish _cMSU _erda |
||
050 | 0 | 0 | _aHF5415.3 JOU |
100 | 1 |
_aChen, Zoey _eauthor |
|
245 | 1 | 0 |
_aWhen, why, and how controversy causes conversation / _ccreated by Zoey Chen and Jonah Berger |
264 | 1 |
_aOxford : _bOxford University Press, _c2013. |
|
336 |
_2rdacontent _atext _btxt |
||
337 |
_2rdamedia _aunmediated _bn |
||
338 |
_2rdacarrier _avolume _bnc |
||
440 |
_aJournal of consumer research _vVolume 40, number 3, |
||
520 | 3 | _aHow does controversy affect conversation? Five studies using both field and laboratory data address this question. Contrary to popular belief, controversial things are not necessarily more likely to be discussed. Controversy increases likelihood of discussion at low levels, but beyond a moderate level of controversy, additional controversy actually decreases likelihood of discussion. The controversy-conversation relationship is driven by two countervailing processes. Controversy increases interest (which increases likelihood of discussion) but simultaneously increases discomfort (which decreases likelihood of discussion). Contextual factors such as anonymity and whether people are talking to friends or strangers moderate the controversy-conversation relationship by impacting these component processes. Our framework sheds light on how, when, and why controversy affects whether or not things are discussed. | |
650 |
_aWord of mouth _vControversy _xConversation |
||
700 | 1 |
_aBerger, Jonah _eco author |
|
856 | _uhttps://doi.org/10.1086/671465 | ||
942 |
_2lcc _cJA |
||
999 |
_c169125 _d169125 |