000 | 01901nam a22002537a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | ZW-GwMSU | ||
005 | 20241217083454.0 | ||
008 | 241217b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
022 | _a00935301 | ||
040 |
_aMSU _bEnglish _cMSU _erda |
||
050 | 0 | 0 | _aHF5415.3 JOU |
100 | 1 |
_aLembregts, Christophe _eauthor |
|
245 | 1 | 0 |
_aAre all units created equal? : the effect of default units on product evaluations/ _ccreated by Christophe Lembregts and Mario Pandelaere |
264 | 1 |
_aOxford : _bOxford University Press, _c2013. |
|
336 |
_2rdacontent _atext _btxt |
||
337 |
_2rdamedia _aunmediated _bn |
||
338 |
_2rdacarrier _avolume _bnc |
||
440 |
_aJournal of consumer research _vVolume 39, number 6, |
||
520 | 3 | _aPrevious research on attribute framing has shown that people often infer higher quantity from larger numbers, usually with the assumption that the units used to specify this information elicit the same meanings. Drawing on literature on categorization and numerical cognition, the authors challenge this assumption and show that consumers often have preset units for attribute levels that strike an optimal balance between a preference for small numbers and the need for accuracy (study 1a). As such, these default units appear commonly (study 1b). Specifying positive attributes in default units renders products’ evaluation more favorable, even if such specification lowers the nominal value of the attributes (studies 2–4). This effect disappears if participants attribute metacognitive feelings generated by default units to an irrelevant source (study 3). Study 5 shows that a default unit effect is more likely in single evaluation mode, but a numerosity effect may reemerge in joint evaluations. | |
650 |
_aConsumer behaviour _vBrand image _xConsumer judgement |
||
700 | 1 |
_aPandelaere, Mario _eco author |
|
856 | _uhttps://doi.org/10.1086/668533 | ||
942 |
_2lcc _cJA |
||
999 |
_c168929 _d168929 |