000 | 01927nam a22002777a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | ZW-GwMSU | ||
005 | 20241206075029.0 | ||
008 | 241206b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
022 | _a0144-3410 | ||
040 |
_aMSU _bEnglish _cMSU _erda |
||
050 | 0 | 0 | _aLB1051 EDU |
100 | 1 |
_aCherney, Isabelle D. _eauthor |
|
245 | 1 | 0 |
_aChildren’s drawings: _ba mirror to their minds _ccreated by Isabelle D. Cherney, Claire S. Seiwert, Tara M. Dickey and Judith D. Flichtbeil |
264 | 1 |
_aOxfordshire: _bTaylor and Francis, _c2005. |
|
336 |
_2rdacontent _atext _btxt |
||
337 |
_2rdamedia _aunmediated _bn |
||
338 |
_2rdacarrier _avolume _bnc |
||
440 |
_aAn international journal of experimental educational psychology _vVolume 26, number 1, |
||
520 | 3 | _aChildren's drawings are thought to be a mirror of a child's representational development. Research suggests that with age children develop more complex and symbolic representational strategies and reference points become more differentiated by gender. We collected two drawings from 109 5-13 year-old children (three age groups). Each child drew their family and their school and participated in an independent recall task. The results indicated significant gender and age differences in the number of details depicted in the family drawings. There were also significant differences between boys' and girls' stereotyped drawings, usage of proportionality, and clothing. With age, children tended to draw more aerial views of their school. The results are discussed in terms of the contribution children's drawings can make to the study of cognitive development and vice versa, as well as their importance for education. | |
650 |
_aChildren _vCognitive development _xArt Activities |
||
700 | 1 |
_aSeiwert, Claire S. _eco-author |
|
700 | 1 | _aDickey, Tara M. | |
700 | 1 | _aFlichtbeil, Judith D. | |
856 | _uhttps://doi.org/10.1080/01443410500344167 | ||
942 |
_2lcc _cJA |
||
999 |
_c168611 _d168611 |