000 | 01900nam a22002777a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | ZW-GwMSU | ||
005 | 20240318135601.0 | ||
008 | 240316b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
022 | _a00222186 | ||
040 |
_aMSU _bEnglish _cMSU _erda |
||
050 | 0 | 0 | _aHB73 JOU |
100 | 1 |
_aBushway, Shawn D. _eauthor |
|
245 | 1 | 0 |
_aFraming punishment : _bincarceration, recommended sentences, and recidivism _ccreated by Shawn D. Bushway and Emily G. Owens |
264 | 1 |
_aChicago: _bUniversity of Chicago Press, _c2013 |
|
336 |
_2rdacontent _atext _btxt |
||
337 |
_2rdamedia _aunmediated _bn |
||
338 |
_2rdacarrier _avolume _bnc |
||
440 |
_aJournal of Law and Economics _vVolume 56, number 2 |
||
520 | 3 | _aNo consensus has emerged about how, or even if, incarceration affects the behavior of convicted offenders. One unexplored mechanism involves the possibility that the disutility of punishment is affected by both the actual punishment an offender receives and the sentence that he thinks could have been given, a psychological effect known as framing. We test for framing effects in punishment by exploiting a legal change in Maryland that altered recommended, but not actual, sentences for a subset of offenders. Using an individual-level data set of convictions, incarceration, and arrests, we find that longer recommended sentences are associated with higher rates of recidivism, conditional on actual punishment. Our results suggest that large discrepancies between the “bark” and “bite” of the criminal justice system may make incarceration less effective at reducing crime | |
650 |
_aCredit for time served _vCriminal behavior _xCriminal justice |
||
650 |
_aCriminal offenses _vCriminal punishment _xCriminal sentencing |
||
650 |
_aCriminals _vRecidivism _xSentenced offenders |
||
700 | 1 |
_aOwens, Emily G. _eco author |
|
856 | _uhttps://doi.org/10.1086/669715 | ||
942 |
_2lcc _cJA |
||
999 |
_c164376 _d164376 |