000 | 02046nam a22002297a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | ZW-GwMSU | ||
005 | 20221104134740.0 | ||
008 | 221104b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
040 |
_aMSU _cMSU _erda |
||
100 |
_aRetelsdorf, Jan _eauthor |
||
245 |
_a“Michael can’t read!” Teachers’ gender stereotypes and boys’ reading self-concept. _ccreated by Jan Retelsdorf, Schwartz, K., & Asbrock, F |
||
264 |
_aGermany _bAmerican Psychological Association _c2014 |
||
336 |
_2rdacontent _atext _btxt |
||
337 |
_2rdamedia _aunmediated _bn |
||
338 |
_2rdacarrier _avolume _bnc |
||
440 | _vVolume , number , | ||
520 | _aAccording to expectancy-value theory, the gender stereotypes of significant others such as parents, peers, or teachers affect students’ competence beliefs, values, and achievement-related behavior. Stereotypically, gender beliefs about reading favor girls. The aim of this study was to investigate whether teachers’ gender stereotypes in relation to reading—their belief that girls outperform boys—have a negative effect on the reading self-concept of boys, but not girls. We drew on a longitudinal study comprising two occasions of data collection: toward the beginning of Grade 5 (T1) and in the second half of Grade 6 (T2). Our sample consisted of 54 teachers and 1,358 students. Using multilevel modeling, controlling for T1 reading self-concept, reading achievement, and school track, we found a negative association between teachers’ gender stereotype at T1 and boys’ reading self-concept at T2, as expected. For girls, this association did not yield a significant result. Thus, our results provide empirical support for the idea that gender differences in self-concept may be due to the stereotypical beliefs of teachers as significant others. In concluding, we discuss what teachers can do to counteract the effects of their own gender stereotypes. | ||
650 | _agender sterotypes | ||
650 | _areading self - concept | ||
856 | _u https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037107 | ||
942 |
_2lcc _cJA |
||
999 |
_c160078 _d160078 |